View original document

The full text on this page is automatically extracted from the file linked above and may contain errors and inconsistencies.

Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago

Teachers and Student Achievement in
the Chicago Public High Schools
Daniel Aaronson, Lisa Barrow
and William Sander

Revised February, 2003

WP 2002-28

Teachers and Student Achievement in the Chicago Public High Schools

June 2003

Daniel Aaronson
Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago
Lisa Barrow
Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago
William Sander
DePaul University

Abstract
Using unique administrative data on Chicago public high school students and their teachers, we
are able to estimate the importance of teachers on student mathematical achievement. We find
that teachers are educationally and statistically important. To be sure, sampling variation and
other measurement issues can strongly influence estimates of teacher effects, and, in some cases,
account for much of the dispersion in teacher quality. Even after correcting for these problems,
we find that one semester with a teacher rated two standard deviations higher in quality could
add 0.3 to 0.5 grade equivalents, or 25 to 45 percent of an average school year, to a student's
math score performance. Additionally, our teacher quality ratings remain relatively stable for an
individual instructor over time, are reasonably impervious to controlling for non-math teachers,
and do not appear to be driven by classroom sorting or selective reporting of test scores. After
relating our measured teacher effects to the standard observable characteristics of the instructor,
we find that traditional human capital and demographic measures, including those used for
compensation purposes, explain little of the total variation in teacher quality.
_____________________________________________________________________________________________
We thank the Chicago Public Schools and the Consortium on Chicago School Research at the University of Chicago
for making the data available to us. We are particularly grateful to John Easton and Jenny Nagaoka for their help in
putting together the data and answering our many follow-up questions. We thank Joe Altonji, Dave Card, Julie
Cullen, Rajeev Dehejia, Tom DiCiccio, Eric French, Brian Jacob, Jeff Kling, Steve Rivkin, Ceci Rouse, Doug
Staiger, Dan Sullivan, Chris Taber, seminar participants at the University of Chicago, DePaul University, the
University of Illinois, the Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago, the ILR-Cornell Institute for Labor Market
Policy/Princeton Industrial Relations Section Tenth Annual Policy Conference, and the Urban School Finance
conference at UIC for helpful comments and discussions. The views expressed in this paper are the views of the
authors and are not necessarily those of the Federal Reserve Bank of Chicago or the Federal Reserve System.
Updated versions of this paper are available by contacting the authors at daaronson@frbchi.org or
lbarrow@frbchi.org.

1. Introduction
The Coleman Report (Coleman et al. 1966) broke new ground in the empirical estimation
of education production functions, concluding that family background and peers were more
important than schools and teachers in determining educational outcomes such as test scores and
graduation rates. While research since the Coleman Report generally supports the influence of
family background, substantiation of the importance of other factors, particularly schools and
teachers, has evolved slowly with the release of better data. Today, most researchers agree that
schools and teachers matter.1 However, how much they matter, the degree to which these effects
vary across student populations, and whether measurable characteristics such as teacher
education and experience affect student educational outcomes continue to be of considerable
research and policy interest.
In this study, we use administrative data on students and teachers in Chicago public high
schools to estimate the importance of teachers on student test score gains in mathematics, and
then relate our measured teacher effects to observable characteristics of the instructors. Our data
provide us with a key and unique advantage: the ability to link teachers with students in specific
classrooms. In contrast, many other studies are able to match students to the average teacher in a
grade or school. In addition, the administrative teacher records allow us to separate the effects of
observed teacher characteristics from unobserved aspects of teacher quality.
Consistent with earlier studies, we find that teachers are important inputs in 9th grade
math achievement. However, a certain degree of caution must be exercised in evaluating teacher
1

Literature reviews include Hanushek (1996,1997,2002) and Greenwald, Hedges, and Laine (1996). A brief
sampling of other recent work on teacher effects includes Rivkin, Hanushek, and Kain (2002), Jepsen and Rivkin
(2001), Goldhaber and Brewer (1997), Jacob and Lefgren (2002), Angrist and Lavy (2001), and Rivers and Sanders
(2002). The earliest studies on teacher quality were hampered by data availability and thus often relied on state or
school-level variation. Hanushek, Rivkin and Taylor (1996) show that aggregation can result in flawed estimates of
education production function parameters. Moreover, measurement error is compounded by proxies, such as
student-teacher ratios and average experience, which do not fully capture the role of an instructor.

1

quality, as biases related to measurement, particularly from changes in exam scoring and the
presence of small populations of students used to identify certain teachers, can critically
influence results. Sampling variation, in particular, overstates our measures of teacher dispersion
by up to 50 percent, consistent with an evaluation of North Carolina schools by Kane and Staiger
(2002). Correcting for sampling error suggests that the variance in teacher quality in the Chicago
public high schools is roughly 0.02 to 0.06 grade equivalents. That is, replacing a teacher with
one that is rated two standard deviations higher in quality adds 0.3 to 0.5 grade equivalents, or 25
to 45 percent of an average school year, to a student’s math score performance. Additionally, we
show that our results are not likely to be driven by classroom sorting or selective use of test
scores and that individual teacher ratings are relatively stable over time, reasonably impervious
to controlling for non-math teachers, and consistent across many student subgroups.
Finally, the vast majority of the variation in teacher effects is unexplained by observable
teacher characteristics, including those used for compensation. While some teacher attributes,
notably undergraduate major, are consistently related to our quality measure, they explain at
most 10 percent of the total variation in teacher quality. The teacher attributes come from
administrative data, a subset of which determines teacher compensation. These facts highlight
the disconnect between teacher pay and productivity, the difficulty in developing compensation
schedules that reward teachers for good work based solely on standard administrative data, and
the difficulty in prescribing recruitment strategies for hiring quality teachers.
While our study focuses on only one school district over a three-year period, this district
serves a large population of minority and lower income students, typical of many large urban
districts in the United States. Fifty-five percent of ninth graders in the Chicago public schools are
African-American, 31 percent are Hispanic, and roughly 80 percent receive free or reduced-price

2

school lunch. Similarly, New York City, Los Angeles Unified, Houston Independent School
District, and Philadelphia City serve student populations that are 80 to 90 percent nonwhite and
roughly 70 to 80 percent eligible for free or reduced-price school lunch (Authors’ calculations
based on the Common Core of Data, 2001). Therefore, on these dimensions Chicago is quite
representative of the school systems that are the focus of U.S. education policy.
2. Data and Background on Chicago Public High School Student Performance
The quality of our data is a major strength of this study. Upon agreement with the
Chicago Public Schools (CPS), the Consortium on Chicago School Research at the University of
Chicago provided us with administrative records from the city's public high schools. These
records include all students enrolled and teachers working in 88 CPS high schools from 1996-97
to 1998-99.2 We concentrate on the performance of 9th graders in this paper.
Apart from offering a large sample of urban school children, the CPS administrative
records provide several other useful features that rarely appear together in other studies. First, the
student data include a history of pre-high school test scores that can be used as controls for past
(latent) inputs. Second, classroom schedule detail allows student-teacher matches at a level that
plausibly corresponds with what we think of as a teacher effect. Finally, the teacher records
include specifics about human capital and demographics. These data allow us to decompose the
total teacher effects into unobservable and observable factors, including those relied on for
compensation decisions by the Chicago public school system. Next, we discuss these issues, and
describe a few econometric issues related to each.
A. Test scores

2

Of the 88 schools, 6 are so small that they do not meet criteria on sample sizes that we describe below. These
schools are generally more specialized, serving students who have not succeeded in the regular school programs.

3

Information on multiple test scores is vital as important family background measures,
particularly income and parental education, are unavailable. While there are various ways to
account for the cumulative effect of inputs that we cannot observe, in the results below we rely
on estimating a general form of the value-added model of education production. In particular, we
estimate the relationship between 9th grade math test scores and the variables of interest while
controlling for initial achievement as measured by the 8th grade test score.
Chicago Public Schools administers the Iowa Test of Basic Skills (ITBS) during the
spring in grades 3 through 8 and the Test of Achievement and Proficiency (TAP) exam during
the spring for grades 9 and 11.3 We observe both 8th and 9th grade test scores for the majority of
ninth grade students, as shown in Table 1. The exams are used to measure whether students have
achieved the skills that are appropriate for their grade. In fact, in the CPS a minimum gradeequivalent score on the ITBS is set as a requirement for promotion from 8th to 9th grade.
Restricting the sample to 9th graders is not limiting in terms of sample size, as 27,000 to
30,000 students are available per year. Eighth and 9th grade test score data are reported for
between 75 and 78 percent of the sample, yielding a potential sample of around 64,000 over the
three-year period. Our sample drops to 53,000 when we exclude students without 8th and 9th
grade test scores, those without scores in consecutive school years, and those in the top and
bottom one percent of score gains.4
The administrative files provide data for the math and reading sections of the TAP and
ITBS. Unique student identifiers allow score gains to be computed. Table 2 displays descriptive
statistics on math test scores from 1993 to 2000. Scores are reported as grade equivalents, a

3

TAP testing was mandatory for grades 9 and 11 through 1998. 1999 was a transition year in which 9th, 10th, and
11th graders were tested. Starting in 2000, TAP testing is mandatory for grades 9 and 10.
4
We discuss the effect of sample selection based on missing test score data below.

4

national normalization that assigns grade levels to test score results. For instance, a 9.7 implies
that the student is performing at the level of a typical student in the 7th month of 9th grade.
Since the late 1980s when former Secretary of Education William Bennett called Chicago
Public Schools the “worst in the nation,” substantial effort has been made to improve public
schools in Chicago. Following the reforms, Hess (1999) and Roderick (2001) document some
initial decline in test-score achievement followed by gains, especially in mathematics. This rise
can be seen in table 2. From 1993 to 2000, 9th grade math test scores rose dramatically, such that
the average test score in 2000 is a full year and two-month grade equivalents higher than it was
in 1993. Eighth-grade scores have increased more modestly, from 7.5 in 1993 to 8.0 in 2000.
Girls and boys score similarly on the 8th and 9th grade math tests, with boys scoring about
one month of a grade equivalent higher on the 9th grade test and girls scoring roughly two
months of a grade equivalent higher on the 8th grade test. Low-income students, defined as those
receiving free or reduced-price school lunch, score only 4 months lower on the 8th grade exam
but just over one year lower on the 9th grade math test. Finally, significant racial and ethnic gaps
exist with African-American and Hispanic students scoring between 8 months and one grade
equivalent below whites on the 8th grade math test and roughly two grade equivalents behind
whites on the 9th grade math test. Asian students have the highest average scores on 8th and 9thgrade tests, averaging roughly one year and six months higher on each.
The raw data suggest that racial and income test score gaps rise dramatically between the
8th and 9th grade. While we expect that higher-ability students may gain more in one year of
education than lower-ability students, we also suspect the rising gap may be a function of the
different exams. More generally, we are concerned about how differences in the 8th and 9th grade
test score distributions may lead to misleading teacher effect estimates. In Figure 1, we plot

5

kernel density estimates of the 8th and 9th grade mathematics test scores. The 9th grade scores are
skewed right while the 8th grade test score distribution is much more symmetric. As a
consequence, controlling for 8th grade test scores in the regression of 9th grade test scores on
teacher indicators and other student characteristics may not adequately control for the initial
quality of a particular teacher’s students. This may lead us to conclude that teachers with better
than average students are superior instructors. Throughout the paper, we drop the top and bottom
one percent of the students by change in test scores to partly account for this problem. We also
discuss additional strategies, including using alternative measures of test scores, accounting for
student attributes, and analyzing groups of students by initial ability. 5
Finally, missing test score data may raise concerns about problems with selection.
Approximately 11 percent of 9th graders do not have 8th grade math test scores and 17 percent do
not have a 9th grade score. There are several possible explanations for this outcome: students
might have transferred from another district, did not take the exam, or perhaps simply did not
have scores appearing in the database. According to the administrative records, 86 percent of the
students took the TAP (9th grade) test, and of this group, we observe scores for 98 percent.
Missing data appear more likely for the subset of students who tend to be male, white or
Hispanic, older, and designated as having special education status (and thus exempt from the
test). Convincing exclusion restrictions are not available to adequately assess the importance of
selection of this type.6 However, later in the paper, we show that our quality measure is not

5

The student controls that are available to us are somewhat limited but include sex, race/ethnicity, age, free and
reduced lunch status, and designated guardian. Because of the paucity of family background information, one
strategy we take is to use available address information to match census tract level income, adult education, and
house value into the data.
6
If selection is based on potential test score improvements because, for example, schools and teachers are somehow
gaming the test score system by reporting only the most improved students' outcomes, we could overstate the impact
of teacher quality (e.g. Jacob and Levitt 2001 and Figlio and Getzler 2002). Identification of a selection equation
requires an exclusion restriction that is able to predict the propensity to have a test score in the administrative
records but is not correlated with the educational production function’s error term. There is no obvious candidate.

6

correlated with missing test scores, suggesting that this type of selection or gaming of the system
is not a unduly influencing our measure of teacher quality.
B. Classroom scheduling and sorting
The second important feature of our data is the detailed scheduling that allows us to
construct the complete history of a student’s class schedule while in the CPS high schools. The
data include where (room number) and when (semester and period) a class met, the teacher
assigned, the title of the class, and the level to which it was taught (i.e. AP, regular, etc.).
Furthermore, we know the letter grade received and the number of classroom absences. Because
teachers and students were matched to the same classroom, we believe we have more power to
estimate teacher effects than is commonly available in administrative records where matching
occurs at the school or grade level. Additionally, since we have this information for every
student, we are able to calculate measures of peer characteristics in the classroom.
One natural concern in how we estimate teacher quality is whether there are lingering
influences from the classroom sorting process. That is, the students with the most or least
achievement potential may be purposely placed with certain instructors. The most likely scenario
involves parental lobbying which may be correlated with expected test score gains. But a school
or teacher may also exert influence that results in nonrandom sorting of students.7

One possibility is to take advantage of the clear difference in absences between test takers and nontakers. Absences
is an obvious correlate of test taking since the propensity for being at school must be associated with taking an exam
at school on a given day. But, of course, absences also proxy for ambition, drive, ability, and family circumstances.
Therefore, we used a factor in school absences, distance to school, that might be uncorrelated with unobserved
student ability. Students who live farther from school likely face additional costs associated with getting there.
Since this restriction is not appropriate if distance proxies for latent aspects of a family that is willing to travel
farther for their school of choice (Cullen, Jacob, and Levitt 2000), we also use a subsample who do not opt out of
their neighborhood school. The actual measure used is a three-threshold spline in distance from the student's census
tract to the school's census tract. We have also tried distance polynomials of various orders but found it made little
difference. These distance variables appear to be useful predictors of the likelihood of 9th grade test score
information being available. Point estimates on the Mill’s ratio suggest that selection may be positively associated
with achievement. Yet, our primary inferences are unaffected by this correction.
7
Informal discussions with a representative of the Chicago public school system suggest that parents have little
influence on teacher selection and the process is not based on characteristics of the students, conditional on course

7

To evaluate the extent to which students may be sorted based on expected test score
gains, we calculate average test score dispersion for the observed teacher assignments and for
several counterfactual teacher assignments. In Table 3, we report the degree to which actual
within-teacher variance in student pre-9th grade performance differs from simulated classrooms
that are either assigned randomly or based on test score rank. We use three lagged test score
measures for assignment: 8th grade test scores, 6th to 7th grade test score gains, and 7th to 8th grade
test score gains. Each panel reports results for the three fall semesters in our data.8 The top row
of each panel, labeled “observed,” displays the observed average within-teacher variance of these
measures. This is the baseline to which we compare the simulations. Each of the four
subsequent rows assigns students to teachers based on pre-9th grade performance characteristics.
Row (2) displays the average within-teacher variance when students are perfectly sorted
across teachers within their original school.9 Such a within-school sorting mechanism reduces
the within-teacher variance to roughly one-third of the observed analog. In contrast, if we
randomly assign students to classrooms within their original school, as shown in row (3), the
average within-teacher variance is very close to the observed within-teacher variance. There is
virtually no evidence that sorting occurs on past gains, with the observed and simulated
variances within 2 percent of each other. The randomly assigned classrooms based on 8th grade
scores tend to have within-teacher variances that are 9 to 10 percent higher than the observed
classrooms. But clearly, the observed teacher variance in lagged math scores is much closer to
what we would expect with random sorting of students than what we would expect if students

level. Furthermore, our use of first-year high school students alleviates concerns since it may be difficult to evaluate
new students, particularly on unobservable characteristics.
8
The estimates for the spring semester are very similar.
9
For example, within an individual school, there may be 10 teachers, each with classrooms of 20 students. In the
simulation, the top 20 students, based on our three pre-9th grade measures, would be placed together, the next 20
together, and so forth. The number of teachers and schools, as well as any heterogeneity in classroom size is set
equal to that observed in the data.

8

were sorted based on their past test performance.10 Thus, we are more confident that teacher
assignment is close to random and less likely to confound our estimates of teacher effects.
C. Teacher records
Finally, we match student administrative records to teacher administrative records using
school identifiers and eight-character teacher codes from the student data.11 The administrative
teacher file contains information on 6,890 teachers in CPS high schools between 1997 and 1999.
Although these data do not provide information on courses taught, through the student files, we
isolate 1,243 possible mathematics and computer science teachers. This list is further pared by
excluding teachers who did not have at least 15 student-semesters during our sample period.
These teachers are placed in the same “other” teacher group for estimation. Ultimately, we
identify teacher effects for 856 math or computer science instructors, as well as an average effect
for those placed in the “other” category. While the student and teacher samples are not as big as
those used in some other administrative files, they allow for reasonably precise estimation.
Matching student and teacher records allows us to take advantage of the third feature of
the data: the detailed demographic and human capital information supplied from the teacher
administrative files. In particular, we can use a teacher's gender, ethnicity, experience, tenure,
university attended, college major, advanced degree achievement, and teaching certification to
decompose total teacher effects into those related to common observable traits of teachers and
those, such as drive, passion, connection with students, and so forth, that are unobserved.
Table 4 provides descriptive statistics of characteristics of the 645 teachers we can match
to the administrative records. The average teacher is 45 years old and has been in the CPS for

10

These calculations are done using all levels of courses—honors, basic, regular, etc. Because most classes are
“regular,” the results are very similar when we limit the analysis to regular level classes.
11
Details about the matching are available in the data appendix. As is made clear there, we cannot match all teacher
codes in the student data to teacher names in the teacher files.

9

13.3 years. Minority math and computer science teachers are underrepresented relative to the
student population, as 37 percent are African-American and 9 percent Hispanic. Almost 85
percent are certified to teach high school, 38 percent are certified to be a substitute, and 10 to 12
percent are certified to teach bilingual, elementary, or special education classes. The majority of
math teachers have a Master’s degree and many report a major in mathematics (47 percent) or
education (19 percent).12
3. Basic Empirical Strategy
In the standard education production function, achievement, Y , of student i with teacher j
in school k at time t is expressed as a function of cumulative own, family, and peer inputs, X,
from age 0 to the current age, as well as, cumulative teacher and school inputs, S, from grades
kindergarten through the current grade:
(1)

T

T

t = −5

t =0

Yijkt = β ∑ X it + γ ∑ S ijkt + ε ijkt

The requirements to estimate (1) are substantial. Without a complete set of conditioning
variables for X and S, omitted variables may bias estimates of the coefficients on observable
inputs unless strong and unlikely assumptions about the covariance structure of observables and
unobservables are maintained. Thus, alternative identification strategies are typically applied.
A simple approach is to take advantage of multiple test scores. In particular, we estimate
a general form of the value-added model by including 8th grade test scores as a covariate. Lagged
test scores account for the cumulative inputs of prior years while allowing for a flexible
autoregressive relationship in test scores. Controlling for past test scores is especially important
with this data, as information on the family and pre-9th grade schooling is sparse.

12

Nationally, 55 percent of high school teachers have a Master’s degree, 66 percent have an academic degree (e.g.
mathematics major), and 29 percent have a subject area education degree (U.S. Department of Education 2000).

10

The education production model is of the general form:
(2)

Y
= αY
+ β X + τTi + θ + µ + ρ + ε
ijkt
ijkt − 1
it
i
t
k
ijkt

where θ , µ , ρ and ε
measure the unobserved impact of individuals, time, schools, and
i t
k
ijkt
white noise. Each element of Ti, Tij, equals the number of semester classes taken with teacher j
in 9th grade. τ j is the jth element of the vector τ and represents the effect of one semester spent
with teacher j in a math or computer science class. Relative to equation (1), the impacts of
lagged schooling and other characteristics are now captured in the lagged test score measure.
This strategy may still mismeasure teacher quality, however. For simplicity, assume that all
students have only one teacher for one semester so that the number of student semesters for
teacher j equals the number of students for teacher j, Nj. In this case, estimates of τ j may be

1
biased by ρ +
k Nj

Nj

∑θ +
i =1

i

1
Nj

Nj

∑ ε ijkt .13
i =1

The school term ρ k is typically removed by including measures of the school quality, a
common and general form of which is school fixed effects. School fixed effect estimation is
useful to control for time-invariant school characteristics that covary with individual teacher
quality, without having to attribute the school’s contribution to specific measures. However,
this strategy requires the identification of teacher effects to be based on differences in the
number of semesters spent with a particular teacher and teachers that switch schools during our
three-year period. For short time periods, such as a single year, there may be little identifying
variation to work with. Thus, this cleaner measure of the contribution of mathematics teachers

13

The time effects are easily captured by year indicators and therefore are not discussed further.

11

comes at the cost of potentially much identifying variation. For that reason, we show many
results without allowing for school fixed effects.
Factors affecting test scores are often attributed to a student’s family background. In the
context of gains, however, time-invariant qualities are differenced out, leaving only factors that
are changing, such as divorce or a student’s introduction to drugs, in

1 Nj
∑ θ . Furthermore,
N j i =1 i

students must be assigned to teachers based on these changes in order to bias our teacher quality
estimates.14 Nevertheless, we test the robustness of our results to the inclusion of observable
student, family, and peer traits because they may be correlated with behavioral changes that
influence achievement and may account for group differences in gain trajectory, thus easing
concerns about test score normalizations.
Finally, as the findings of Kane and Staiger (2002) make clear, the error term
1 Nj
is particularly troubling when fixed effect estimates are based on small populations
∑ε
N j i =1 ijkt
(small N j ). In this case, sampling variation can overwhelm signal, causing a few good or bad
draws to strongly influence the estimated teacher fixed effect. Consequently, the variance of the
distribution of estimated τ is most likely inflated.
j
This problem is illustrated in Figure 2. The chart computes τ

j

conditional on 8th grade

math score, year indicators, and student, family, and peer attributes, as described below. What is
notable is that the lowest and highest performing teachers are those with the fewest student

14

We do not discount the possibility of this type of sorting, especially for transition schools, which are available to
students close to expulsion. School fixed effects pick this up but we also estimate the results excluding these
schools.

12

semesters.

∑T

ij

represents the number of student semesters taught by teacher j over the time

i

period examined. As more student semesters are used to estimate the fixed effect, the
importance of sampling variation declines and reliability improves. Regressing | τ j |
on ∑ Tij summarizes this association. Such an exercise has a coefficient estimate of -0.00047
i

with a standard error of 0.00008, suggesting that number of student semesters is a critical
correlate of the magnitude estimated teacher quality. The association declines as we raise the
minimum threshold on

∑T

ij .

Statistical significance disappears when

i

∑T

ij

≥ 200 .

i

To address the problem of sampling error, we analytically adjust the variance of τˆ for
j
the size of the sampling error by assuming that the estimated teacher fixed effect is the sum of
the actual teacher effect, τ j , plus some error. We use the mean of the square of the standard
error estimates of τˆ j as an estimate of the sampling variance and subtract this from the observed
variance of τˆ j to get an “adjusted” variance. We report both the variance of τˆ j and the
adjusted variance in the tables below. We also show how these values vary as we increase the
minimum evaluation threshold,

∑T

ij

. Sampling error largely disappears when the minimum is

i

set high enough.15
In the section to follow, we present our baseline estimates that ignore the existence of
most of these potential biases. Thus, they should be considered naïve. We then report results
that attempt to deal with each potential bias. To the extent that real world evaluation might not

15

Note, however, that excluding teachers with small numbers of students is limiting because new teachers,
particularly those for whom tenure decisions are being considered, cannot be examined.

13

account for these problems, this exercise could be considered a cautionary tale of the extent to
which teacher quality estimates can be interpreted incorrectly.
Finally, we examine whether teacher quality can be explained by demographic and
human capital attributes of teachers. Because of concerns raised by Moulton (1986) about the
efficiency of OLS estimates in the presence of a school-specific fixed effect and because students
are assigned multiple teachers per year, we do not include the teacher characteristics directly in
equation (2). Rather, we employ a GLS estimator outlined in Borjas (1987) and Borjas and
Sueyoshi (1994). This estimator regresses τˆ j on teacher characteristics Z:

τˆ j = φZ j + u j ;

(3)

The variance of the errors is calculated as the covariance matrix derived from OLS estimates of
(3) and the portion of equation (2)’s variance matrix related to the τˆ coefficient estimates, V.
Ω = σ u2 I J + V

(4)

The Ω term in (4) is used to compute GLS estimates of the observable teacher effects.
4. Results

A. The distribution of teacher quality
Our naïve baseline estimates of teacher quality are presented in table 5. In column (1) we
present details on distribution of τˆ , specifically the variance and the 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and
j
90th percentiles. We also list the p-value for an F-test of the joint significance of the teacher
effects (i.e. τ k = 0 for all k) and the p-value for an F-test of the other regressors. Since this is
our most parsimonious specification, the list of regressors is limited to year dummies and the 8th
grade math score.16 Clearly, we cannot rule out the importance of confounding changes in family,
16

Naturally, the key covariate in our production functions, regardless of specification, is the 8th grade test score.
The t-statistic on this variable often exceeds 200. Yet the magnitude of the point estimate is somewhat surprising, in

14

student, peer, and school influences, as well as random fluctuations in student performance
across teachers. Rather, we report these estimates as a baseline for considering the importance of
these biases.
Consequently, the estimated range of the teacher fixed effects is quite broad, perhaps
implausibly so. The variance of τˆ is 0.21 with gaps between the 90th percentile and 10th
j
percentile teacher of over 1 grade equivalent. Furthermore, approximately 0.47 grade
equivalents separate average class gains between the 75th and 25th percentile teacher. An F-test
of the joint significance of τˆ easily rejects no teacher effects at the highest significance level.
j
The robustness of these results can be explored by tracking the stability of individual
teacher quality over time. To do so, we reestimate equation (2) but with t subscripts on τ j .17 In
table 6 we display the resulting transition matrix linking quartile rankings of τˆ
rankings of τˆ

jt + 1

jt

with quartile

. Quartile 1 represents the lowest 25 percent of teachers, as ranked by the

teacher effect estimate, and quartile 4 the highest 25 percent. The table reports each cell’s share
of a row’s total or the fraction of teachers in quartile q in year t that move to each of the four
quartiles in year t+1. If our estimates are consistent with some signal, whether it is quality or
something correlated with quality, we would expect masses of teachers on the diagonals of the

that it is often greater than 1. For example, in our sparsest specification, the coefficient on 8th grade test score is
1.30 (0.01). This suggests the math test score time-series may not be stationary. However, this is not likely to be a
problem since we are working off of the cross-section. It would become an issue if we were to include longitudinal
information on 10th or 11th grade. Nevertheless, a simple way to deal with nonstationarity is to estimate equation (3)
in differenced form:

Yijkt − Yijkt −1 = α (Yit −1 − Yit − 2 ) + β ( X it − X it −1 ) + γ (Wikt − Wikt −1 ) + τ (Tit − Tit −1 ) + ε ijt − ε ijt −1

Such a specification will lead to inconsistent estimates because of the correlation between the error term with the
lagged differenced dependent variable, but a common strategy to avoid this problem is to use the twice lagged
differenced dependent variable, Yit − 2 − Yit − 3 as an instrument. This IV estimator broadly supports the results

presented below.
Of course, this amplifies sampling variability.

17

15

transition matrix where quality quartiles are constant across years. We expect cells farther from
the diagonals to be monotonically less common. Particularly noisy estimates would not be able
to reject the pure random assignment result that each cell would contain equal shares of teachers.
In this rather extreme case, teachers would be randomly assigned a new quality ranking each
year, and the correlation between this year’s ranking and next would be 0.
Our results suggest a nontransitory component to the teacher quality measure. Of the
teachers in the lowest quality quartile in year t, 40 percent remain in year t+1, 28 percent move
into quartile 2, 24 percent into quartile 3 and 8 percent into the highest quartile. Of those in the
highest quartile in year t (row 4), 60 percent remain the following year, 24 percent move one
category down, and only 16 percent fall to the lowest two quartiles. A chi-square test easily
rejects random assignment for each row.18
Moreover, we also explored to what extent teachers in the top and bottom deciles of the
quality distribution continue to rank there the following year. Of the teachers in the top decile, 46
percent rank there the following year. This is highly significant relative to the random draw
scenario whereby 10 percent would again appear in the top decile in consecutive years.
However, of those teachers in the bottom decile, only 14 percent remain there the following year.
Given our sample sizes, this is not significantly different from the random assignment baseline.
We believe the latter result is partly driven by greater turnover among teachers in the
bottom decile. By definition, to appear in our transition matrix, a teacher must be in the
administrative records for two consecutive years. However, our teacher distributions are derived
from the full population. Therefore, if poor performing teachers are more likely to leave the
school system, this could bias our test; the random draw baseline would no longer be 10 percent.

16

To investigate this possibility, we regress an indicator of whether the teacher appears in the
teacher records in year t+1 on whether she is ranked in the top or bottom decile of the quality
distribution in year t.19 We find that a teacher ranked at the bottom is 26 percent less likely
(standard error of 4 percent) than a teacher ranked in the 10th to 90th percentile to appear in the
administrative records the following year. In contrast, teacher turnover for those in the top decile
is no different than turnover for the middle group. Once we account for this turnover behavior,
the share of teachers remaining in the bottom decile of the teacher quality distribution is
significant at standard levels.20
These results emphasize that teacher quality evaluated using parsimonious specifications
with little attention to measurement issues still has an important persistent component. In fact,
we find it encouraging that there is any signal to gauge. However, the transitory part, which is
aggravated by sampling error when looking at estimates based on one year, is also apparent.
Furthermore, the magnitude of the estimates is perhaps improbably large.
B. The impact of sampling error
We next consider how sampling error may affect our results. We already attempt to
improve the signal-to-noise ratio by throwing out students with grade changes in the extreme
tails and by restricting identified teachers to those with more than 15 student semesters.
However, Kane and Staiger (2002) show that more than half of the variance in score gains from
small North Carolina schools, which tend to be smaller than our

∑T

ij ,

and one-third of the

i

18

Similarly, regressing contemporaneous teacher quality on lagged teacher quality results in a point estimate of 0.44
(0.05) for 1998 and 0.53 (0.06) for 1999. Limiting it to teachers in all three years, the coefficients (and standard
errors) on lagged and twice lagged teacher quality are 0.48 (0.08) and 0.31 (0.08).
19
Unfortunately, we cannot distinguish quits and layoffs, nor exits out of teaching from exits into other school
systems.
20
The adjustment assumes that the share of teachers that remain at the bottom decile under the random draw
baseline is 7.4 percentage points, or 26 percent lower than 10 percentage points.

17

variance in that state’s larger schools are due to sampling variation. Figure 2 emphasizes the
susceptibility of our results to these concerns as well.
The row labeled “adjusted variance” in table 5 presents an estimate of the variance of τ j
after adjusting for sampling variation as described above. This modification reduces the variance
from 0.208 to 0.172, suggesting that 17 percent of the variation in teacher quality arises from
sampling error. We can confirm this result simply by adjusting for possible overweighting of
unreliable observations. Column (2) reports the distribution of τˆ j , when weighted by

∑T

ij

.

i

The weighted variance of the teacher effects drops to 0.155, comparable in size to the adjusted
variance reported in column (1). In either case, the main conclusion remains that dispersion in
teacher quality is wide and educationally significant.21
C. The impact of family, student, and peer characteristics
The results thus far report on specifications that are quite sparse. They do not fully
capture heterogeneity in student, family, and peer background that could be correlated with
particular teachers. Moreover, little has been done to account for variation introduced by exam
normalization differences. To this end, table 7 reports results in which student, family, and peer
group characteristics available in the administrative records are included. For comparison
purposes, column (1) repeats the findings from table 5. Each column reports unadjusted,
adjusted, and weighted variance estimates, as well as p-values for F-tests of the joint significance
of the teacher effects and the other regressors as they are added to the production function.
Column (2) incorporates student characteristics including gender, race, age, designated
guardian relationship (mom, dad, stepparent, other relative, or nonrelative), and free and
21

We also experimented with the common strategy of using 6th grade math scores as an instrumental variable for 8th
grade scores. Classical measurement error in the 9th grade scores affects efficiency not consistency. The F-value on

18

reduced-price lunch eligibility. In addition, we include a measure of the student’s average 9th
grade math class size, as is standard in educational production analysis, as well as controls for
whether the student changed high schools or repeats the 9th grade.22 These controls reduce the
size of the variance by roughly one-third but the estimates remain large and highly significant.
In column (3) we introduce additional student controls, primarily related to performance,
school choice, and peer and neighborhood characteristics. The additional student regressors are
the level and subject matter of math classes, the student’s cumulative grade point average, class
rank, disability status, and whether the school is outside of her residential neighborhood.23 The
neighborhood measures based on Census data for a student’s residential census tract include
median family income, median house value, and the fraction of adults that fall into five
education categories; they are meant to proxy for latent parental influences. Again, like many of
the student controls, the value-added framework should account for permanent income gaps but
will not account for differences in student growth rates by parental income or education. Finally,
the math class peer characteristics include the 10th, 50th, and 90th percentiles of math class

this IV regression is somewhat smaller than in our basic specification but the distribution of the teacher effects is
relatively unchanged.
22
Jointly these background measures are quite significant; individually, the sex and race measures are the primary
drivers. Female students gain 0.16 (0.01) grade equivalents less than males, and black and Hispanic students gain
0.49 (0.03) and 0.30 (0.03) less than nonblack, nonhispanic students. Accounting for student performance,
neighborhood, and peer controls diminishes the racial differences slightly but the female gap nearly doubles.
Students whose designated guardian is the father have, on average, 0.10 to 0.20 higher test score gains than do
students with other guardians. Math class size has a positive and significant relationship with test scores; however,
once we control for additional classroom characteristics, it switches sign and is usually insignificant.
23
As math teachers can influence the student’s study habits and performance outside the math class, our teacher
effect estimates might be biased downward by introducing such controls.
We also experiment with additional controls for student ability, including 8th grade reading scores, 6th and 7th grade
math scores, and the variance in 6th-8th grade math scores. When we control for 8th grade reading scores, the point
estimate on the 8th grade math score declines by about 4 percent but the impact on the teacher effects is minimal.
Including controls for 6th, 7th, and 8th grade math scores distributes the autoregressive component of math test scores
between the three as follows: 0.28 (0.01), 0.42 (0.01), and 0.71 (0.01). However, again, there is no additional effect
on our estimated teacher variances. We have also allowed 8th grade math test scores to enter in alternative formats,
including as a spline and as indicator categories representing four quartiles of performance and indicators of below
and above national norm performance. None of these specification choices are important, relative to the simple
linear 8th grade score control. Finally, including the variance of junior high math scores is an important and
interesting dimension of 9th grade achievement, but has no collateral impact on the teacher estimates.

19

absences, as a means of measuring how disruptive the classroom is, and the same percentiles of
8th grade math test scores as a measure of peer ability. Because teacher ability may influence
classroom attendance patterns, peer absences could confound our estimates of interest, leading to
downward biased teacher quality estimates.24
Adding peer and neighborhood covariates reduces the adjusted variance to 0.059, onethird the size of the naïve estimates reported in column (1).25 Much of the attenuation comes
from adding either own or peer performance measures. Nevertheless, regardless of the controls
introduced, the dispersion in teacher quality remains high and statistically significant. The Fvalue of the joint test of the teacher effects drops below 5, to 4.6, only when the full set of
controls are included, but remains statistically significant at the highest levels.
Once again, transition matrices for the full control specification clearly reject random
quality draws. The quartile-rank matrix is reported in table 8. 40 percent of teachers ranking in
the top 25 percent in one year rank in the top 25 percent in the following year. Another 23
percent slip down one category, 21 percent two categories and 16 percent to the bottom category.
All other rows are similarly monotonic and reject chi-square tests at standard significance
levels.26

24

See Manski (1993) for a methodological discussion and Hoxby (2000) and Sacerdote (2001) for evidence. While
we hesitate to place a causal interpretation on the peer measures, there is a statistical association between a student’s
performance and that of her peers. The point estimates (standard errors) on the 10th, 50th, and 90th percentile of peer
absences are 0.006 (0.005), -0.006 (0.002), and -0.005 (0.001). Including the level and subject of the class and own
student’s overall performance eliminates the influence of the median peer and cuts the 90th percentile student’s
influence in half. Thus it appears that the main effect is from missed class among the most absent of students. The
point estimates on the 10th, 50th, and 90th percentile of 8th grade math scores are 0.052 (0.014), 0.205 (0.025), and
0.162 (0.020). These peer measures reduce the student’s own 8th grade math test score influence by about 10
percent. Including the additional student performance and class type regressors reduces the peer 8th grade score
estimates to 0.025 (0.013), 0.137 (0.025), and 0.117 (0.020), suggesting that high performers have the most
influence on student performance.
25
Arguably, part of the reduction in variance is excessive, as teachers may affect academic performance through an
effect on absences or GPA. That said, eliminating student or peer measures based on 9th grade performance has a
small impact (0.01).
26
18 and 12 percent of those in the top and bottom deciles remain the next year. 22 and 23 percent rated in the top
and bottom deciles in 1997 are still there in 1999. Again, turnover is high among the lowest performing teachers.

20

D. Within-School Estimates
Within-school variation in teacher quality is often preferred to the between-school variety
as it eliminates concerns about school-level factors, including the principal, curriculum, school
size or composition, quality of other teachers in the school, and latent family or neighborhoodlevel characteristics that might influence school choice. Because our results are based on
achievement gains, we are generally concerned only with changes in these factors. However,
school fixed-effect estimation minimizes potential bias caused by school choice, as only changes
in parental demand for school quality confound our teacher estimates. Since all our students are
in new schools in the 9th grade, many of the school and institutional factors may be relevant.
Therefore, restricting the source of teacher variation to within-school differences will result in a
more consistent, but less efficient, measure of the contribution of teachers.
Our primary method of controlling for school-level influences is school fixed effects
estimation. As mentioned above, identification depends on differences in the intensity of teacher
use by students within-schools as well as teachers switching schools during the sample period.
We report these results in columns (4) and (5) of table 7. Relative to the analogous columns
without school fixed effects the dispersions in teacher quality are similar, although variance
magnitude and the precision of the estimates decline somewhat. The correlation of τ j with and
without school fixed effects is 0.87. With the full set of controls, the adjusted variance drops
from 0.059 (column 3) to 0.040 (column 5), implying that a two standard deviation improvement
in teacher quality produces, on average, a 0.4 grade-equivalent test score gain; 0.09 grade
equivalents less than the estimate without school fixed effects. Again, an F-test rejects that the
within-school teacher quality estimates jointly equal zero at the 1-percent level, although the
level of the F-statistic drops to 2.7.

21

School fixed effect estimation has the distinct advantage of not having to attribute school
performance to specific measurable characteristics. Because we are concerned about the loss of
identifying variation, however, we alternatively tried controlling for additional characteristics.
The controls include the student’s 8th and/or 9th grade reading scores, as well as school-level
characteristics—average 9th grade math and reading scores, average number of absences, the size
of the school, and the type of school (neighborhood, selective, charter, alternative, special
education)—to proxy for school quality. Controlling for students’ 8th and 9th grade reading
scores or just 9th grade reading scores lowers the adjusted variance in teacher quality to 0.047,
relatively close to the 0.040 school fixed effects estimate. In contrast, school-level composition
controls—school size, average 8th grade math and reading scores, average number of absences,
and school type—lower the adjusted variance only to 0.051. These results suggest that aggregate
school measures, which are commonly used in the older literature, may be a less satisfactory way
to proxy for the school-level effects.
E. Additional Robustness Checks
This section provides additional evidence on the robustness of our results to the gaming
of score reporting, sampling variability, test normalization, and the inclusion of other teachers in
the math score production function.
Cream skimming
One concern is that teachers or schools discourage some students from taking exams
because they are expected to perform poorly. For example, a teacher could increase test
performance, and thus her quality ranking, by only having the best students take the exam. In
order to evaluate whether this may be occurring, we explored how our estimate of teacher quality
relates to the share of a teacher’s students missing 8th or 9th grade test scores. In both cases, the

22

correlation is low (-0.04), opposite in sign to the cream skimming prediction, and not statistically
significant.
Another way to game exam results is for teachers or schools to test students whose scores
are not required to be reported and then report scores for those students who do well. To examine
this possibility, we calculate the correlation between teacher quality and the share of students
excluded from exam reporting. In this case, evidence is consistent with gaming of scores; the
correlation is positive (0.08) and statistically significant at the 5 percent level. To gauge the
importance of this finding for our results, we reran our statistical models dropping all students
for whom test scores may be excluded from school and district reporting. This exclusion affected
6,371 students (12 percent of the full sample) but had no substantive impact on our results.
Sampling variability: Restrictions on student semester observations
A simple strategy for minimizing sampling variability is to restrict evaluation to teachers
with a large number of student semesters. In table 9, we explore limiting assessment of teacher
dispersion to teachers with at least 50 or 100 student semesters. We emphasize that a sampling
restriction, while useful for its simplicity, can be costly in terms of inference. Obviously, the
number of teachers for whom we can estimate quality is reduced. There may also be an issue
about how representative the teachers are particularly since we overlook an important sample of
teachers, namely new instructors with upcoming tenure decisions. Finally, sampling variation
exists with large numbers of students as well, so we would not expect to offset concerns about
measurement error completely by simply setting a high minimum

∑T

ij

.

i

Columns (1) and (2) report results further increasing the minimum student semesters
required to estimate teacher quality. In Panel A we include all covariates from the specification
presented in column 3 of table 7. Panel B additionally includes school fixed effects. Using a 50

23

or 100 student-semester threshold and the full control specification, we find that the adjusted
variance is roughly 0.04 without school fixed effects and 0.02 grade equivalents with school
fixed effects. In both cases, the teacher effects are jointly statistically significant. Note that
increasing the minimum student semesters from 15 to 100 increases the average number of
student semesters per teacher from 106 to 196. Consequently, sampling variability drops from
0.032 grade equivalents (0.092 minus 0.059) for the 15 student threshold to 0.007 (0.049 minus
0.042) for the 100 student threshold.
More on test score normalization and the undue influence of outliers
The remaining columns of table 9 include several additional attempts to minimize the
influence of outlier observations. Column (3) reports findings using national percentile rankings
that are impervious to the normalization problem inherent in grade equivalent scores.27 We find
that the adjusted variance of τˆ j is 4.25 percentile points, a result that is statistically and
economically significant, broadly consistent with the grade equivalent results, and robust to
exploiting only within-school variation.28
In the next column we simply trim the top and bottom 3 percent of the distribution of 8th
to 9th grade math test gains from the student sample. We would clearly expect that this sample
restriction would reduce the variance, as it eliminates roughly 2,600 students in the tails of the
score distribution. Still, the adjusted teacher variance remains large in magnitude and
statistically significant at 0.042 grade equivalents.29

27

These rankings have the advantage of potentially greater consistency across tests so long as the reference
population of test takers is constant. The publisher of the tests, Riverside Publishing, advertises the TAP as being
“fully articulated” with the ITBS and useful for tracking student progress.
28
Just under 2 percent of the sample is left or right censored, of which over 98 percent are at the lowest possible
percentile score of 1 Estimates using a tobit to account for this censoring problem result in virtually identical
coefficient estimates and estimates of the variance of the τˆ j .
29

We have also estimated the education production function using the robust estimator developed by Huber to
account for outliers. The technique weights observations based on an initial regression and is useful for its high

24

Finally, we stratify the sample into ability groups based on 8th grade math test score and
re-estimate the teacher effects within ability group.30 Low ability students are defined as those in
the bottom third of the Chicago public school 8th grade score distribution, at or below 7.5 grade
equivalents. As reported in the final three columns of table 9, low ability students have a mean
9th grade score of 7.1 and a mean test score change of 0.54. High ability students are in the top
third of the 8th grade test score distribution with scores above 8.7 (i.e. performing at or above
national norms). These students have mean 9th grade scores of 11.8 and mean test score growth
of 2.2 grade equivalents. All other students are classified as “middle” ability. The middle group
has an average 9th grade test score of 8.7 grade equivalents and a mean change of 0.67. Looking
at subgroups of students with more similar initial test scores should help reduce the possibility
that teacher effect estimates are simply measuring test score growth related to normalization
issues. As such, it can be considered another test of the robustness of the results. Moreover, it is
of independent interest to document the effect of teachers on different student populations,
particularly those achieving at the lowest and highest levels. The major drawback, of course, is
that by limiting the sample to a particular subgroup we exacerbate the small sample size problem
in estimating teacher quality.
Among all ability groups, we attribute large shares of the variance in estimated teacher
effects to sampling variability. That said, a two standard deviation improvement in teacher
quality is still worth a sizable gain in average test score growth, particularly among the middle
and low achieving populations. A two standard deviation increase in teacher quality for one
semester raises 9th grade test score performance by 0.28, 0.48, and 0.43 grade equivalents for
degree of efficiency in the face of heavy-tailed data. These results generate an even wider distribution of estimated
teacher quality and are not reported in the paper.

25

low, middle, and high ability students. These are 52, 71, and 19 percent of average test score
gains from 8th to 9th grade for each group.31
Including other teachers in the production function
We explore one final specification that takes advantage of the detailed classroom
scheduling in our data by including a full set of English teacher semester counts, akin to the math
teacher semester count, Ti, in equation (2). Assuming the classroom sorting mechanism is similar
across subject areas (e.g., parents who demand the best math teacher will also demand the best
English teacher or schools will sort students into classrooms and assign classes to teachers based
on the students’ expected test score gains), the English teachers will pick up some sorting that
may confound estimates of τ . Moreover, the English teachers may help us gauge the
importance of teacher externalities, i.e., the proverbial superstar teacher who inspires students to
do well not just in their class but in all classes. In the presence of student sorting by teacher
quality, these spillover effects will exacerbate the bias in the math teacher quality estimates.
Although we cannot separately identify classroom sorting from teacher spillovers, we are
primarily interested in testing the robustness of our math teacher effects to such controls. Recall,
however, that the table 3 results suggest classroom sorting is fairly minimal.
We report estimates including English teachers in table 10. For additional comparison,
we also report variances of the English teacher effect estimates both with and without controls
for the math teachers. Controlling for English teachers, the math teacher adjusted variance is
somewhat smaller and less precisely estimated. That said, the dispersion in English teacher
quality, at least in terms of their effect on math scores, is less than one-third that of math
30

We have also stratified the sample by sex and race. Teacher dispersion is higher for male than female students by
nearly 30 percent. By race/ethnicity, the adjusted variance is 0.056 for African-Americans and 0.055 for Hispanics.
Unfortunately, small sample sizes preclude reliable estimation for other races.

26

teachers. This is consistent with our expectation math teachers are a more important input in
math achievement than are English teachers. Thus, we conclude that our results are robust to
controls for additional teachers. 32
5. Predicting Teacher Quality Based on Resume Characteristics

This final section relates our estimates of τ j to measurable characteristics of the
instructors available in the CPS administrative records. Observable characteristics include
common demographic and human capital characteristics such as teachers’ gender, race, potential
experience, tenure at CPS, advanced degrees (Masters or Ph.D.), undergraduate major,
undergraduate college attended, and teaching certifications. We report select results in table 11.
All are based on the full control specification reported in column 3 of table 7. We discuss
common themes below.
First and foremost, the vast majority of the total variation in teacher quality is
unexplained by observable teacher characteristics. At one extreme, a cubic in tenure and
indicators for advanced degrees and teaching certifications explains at most 3 percent of the total
variation, adjusting for the share of total variation due to sampling error. 33 That is, the
characteristics on which compensation is based have extremely little power in explaining teacher
quality dispersion. Including other teacher characteristics, changing the specifications for

31

Although not related directly to the teacher effects, the dynamics of the test scores differ across groups as well.
The autoregressive component of math scores is substantially lower for the lowest achieving students (around 0.47)
relative to middle and high ability students (1.3 and 1.4).
32
Alternatively, we can substitute reading scores for math scores in a production function with both English and
math teachers. In this case, the adjusted variances for math teachers equals 0.029 and for English teachers equals
0.020. The table 10 results are based on a specification without school fixed effects. Including school fixed effects,
the variance of the math teacher effects is slightly smaller at 0.047, and the variance on the English teacher effects is
somewhat smaller at 0.024. Precision is also affected by the limitation to within-school variation although both the
math and English teacher effects are jointly significant at the 1 percent level.
33
The R2 is an understatement of the explanatory power since up to 50 percent of the variation in τˆ j is due to
sampling error. If we simply multiply the total sum of squares by 50 percent to account for this, the R2 will double.
However, it still remains below 10 percent in all cases.

27

computing the teacher effects, and altering the minimum student-semester threshold have little
impact on this inference. In all cases, the R 2 barely exceeds 0.05.
Given a lack of compelling explanatory power, it is of little surprise that few human
capital regressors are associated with teacher quality. A notable exception is math or science
undergraduate degrees, which are associated with teacher quality of 0.06 to 0.08 grade
equivalents higher.34 The majority of standard education background characteristics, including
certifications, advanced degrees, and graduating from a top university, are loosely, if at all,
related to τˆ .35
j
Experience and tenure have little relation to τ j when introduced in levels (unreported) or
higher order powers. In column (3) we look specifically at teachers with less than one or exactly
one year of potential experience on average over the three years compared to teachers with more
potential experience. Again we find no statistically significant difference. That said, teachers
with less than one year of potential experience are associated with estimated quality that is 0.031

34

Other studies that correlate specific human capital measures to teacher quality are mixed. Hanushek (1971) finds
no relationship between teacher quality and experience or master’s degree attainment. Rivkin, Hanushek, and Kain
(2002) also find no link between education level and teacher quality, although they find a small positive relationship
between the first two years of teacher experience and teacher quality. Summers and Wolfe (1977) find that student
achievement is positively related to the teacher’s undergraduate college while student achievement is negatively
related to the teacher’s test score on the National Teacher Examination test. In contrast, Hanushek (1971) finds that
teacher verbal ability is positively related to student achievement for students from “blue-collar” families. Ferguson
(1998) argues teacher test score performance is the most important predictor of a teacher’s ability to raise student
achievement. Goldhaber and Brewer (1997) find some evidence that teacher certification in mathematics or
majoring in mathematics is positively related to teacher quality. Other work by Jacob and Lefgren (2002) and
Angrist and Lavy (2001) find no evidence that human capital investment in the form of teacher in-service training
influence student achievement.
35
Bilingual certification is associated with lower student gains. However, this result is likely related to the difficulty
of teaching children with English as a second language rather than an indictment of the certificate itself. Our
inability to identify potentially important contributors to achievement such as student’s native language with the data
in hand is a problem with most administrative records. The bilingual result disappears when we just look at withinschool variation suggesting that bilingual students are concentrated in particular schools.
Our data include the name of the undergraduate university. We aggregate universities into six categories,
based on U.S. News and World Reports’ rankings.

28

(standard error of 0.052) grade equivalents lower than teachers with more than one year of
potential experience.36
Finally, the race/ethnicity of the teacher has no significant effect on overall student
achievement although female teachers are associated with test scores roughly 0.045 grade
equivalents higher. Moreover, we find little compelling evidence (unreported) that students
perform better or worse with teachers that “look like them” with the exception of AfricanAmerican male students.37 For African-American male students, African-American teachers are
associated with math test scores that are 0.11 (standard error of 0.04) grade equivalents higher;
there is no statistically significant difference for African-American female students.
6. Conclusion

The primary implication of our results is that teachers matter. While this has been
obvious to those working in the school systems, it is only in the last few years that social
scientists have had access to data necessary to verify and estimate the magnitude of these effects.
In spite of the improved data, the literature remains somewhat in the dark about what makes a
good teacher. Our results are consistent with related studies like Hanushek (1992) and Rivkin,
Hanushek, and Kain (2002) who argue that unobservables are driving much of the dispersion in
teacher quality. Traditional human capital measures have few robust associations with measures
of teacher quality and explain a very small fraction of its wide dispersion. That our teacher
measure has an autoregressive component implies that principals may eventually be able to
identify quality; however, they are unlikely to have information on teacher quality when
recruiting or for recent hires, where quality may be poorly inferred due to sampling variability.
36

Combining the 0 and 1 year categories results in an estimate of -0.011 (0.023). When we include a similarly
constructed measure of tenure, we find no independent effect of being new to the school system.

29

Moreover, while it is often argued that low achievement in Chicago is a result of inadequate
resources (e.g. Kozol 1991), it is unclear that more money would have a large impact unless it is
directed in the proper manner (Hanushek, Kain, and Rivkin 1999). One common proposal is to
tie teacher pay more directly to performance, rather than the current system, which is based on
measures that are unrelated to student achievement, namely, teacher education and tenure. That
said, such a compensation scheme would require that serious attention be paid to important
measurement problems associated with identifying quality.

37

Goldhaber and Brewer (1997) find teacher quality higher among female and lower among African-American
instructors. Ehrenberg, Goldhaber, and Brewer (1995) and Dee (2001) also look at teacher race and/or sex but
instead focus on whether students perform better with teachers of their own race and/or sex.

30

References

Angrist, Joshua and Victor Lavy, 2001, “Does Teacher Training Affect Pupil Learning?
Evidence from Matched Comparisons in Jerusalem Public Schools,” Journal of Labor
Economics 19:343-369.
Borjas, George, 1987, "Self-Selection and the Earnings of Immigrants," American Economic
Review 77:531-553.
Borjas, George and Glenn Sueyoshi, 1994, "A Two-Stage Estimator for Probit Models with
Structural Group Effects," Journal of Econometrics 64:165-182.
Coleman, James S., et al. 1966, Equality of Educational Opportunity, Washington, D.C.: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Cullen, Julie, Brian Jacob, and Steven Levitt, 2000, "The Impact of School Choice on Student
Outcomes: An Analysis of the Chicago Public Schools," Journal of Public Economics,
forthcoming.
Dee, Thomas, 2001, "Teachers, Race, and Student Achievement in a Randomized Experiment,"
working paper, Swarthmore College.
Ehrenberg, Ronald, Daniel Goldhaber, and Dominic Brewer, 1995, “Do teachers’ race, gender,
and ethnicity matter?” Industrial and Labor Relations Review 48:547-561.
Ferguson, Ronald, 1998, “Paying for public education,” Harvard Journal of Legislation 28:465498.
Figlio, David and Larry Getzler, 2002, “Accountability, Ability, and Disability: Gaming the
System,” Working paper, University of Florida.
Goldhaber, Dan and Dominic Brewer, 1997, “Why don’t school and teachers seem to matter?”
Journal of Human Resources 32: 505-523.
Greenwald, Rob, Larry Hedges, and Richard Laine, 1996, “The Effect of School Resources on
Student Achievement,” Review of Educational Research 66:361-396.
Hanushek, Eric, 1971, “Teacher characteristics and gains in student achievement,” American
Economic Review 61: 280-288.
Hanushek, Eric, 1992, “The Trade-off Between Child Quantity and Quality,” Journal of Political
Economy 100:84-117.

31

Hanushek, Eric, 1996, “Measuring Investment in Education,” Journal of Economic Perspectives
10:9-30.
Hanushek, Eric, 1997, “Assessing the effects of school resources on student performance: An
update,” Education Evaluation and Policy Analysis 19:141-164.
Hanushek, Eric, 2002, “Publicly Provided Education,” In Auerbach and Feldstein (eds),
Handbook of Public Finance, Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Hanushek, Eric, Steven Rivkin, and Lori Taylor, 1996, “Aggregation and the Estimated Effects
of School Resources,” Review of Economics and Statistics, 78:611-627.
Hanushek, Eric, John Kain, and Steven Rivkin, 1999, “Do Higher Salaries Buy Better
Teachers?” Working paper, University of Texas at Dallas.
Hess, G. 1999, “Understanding Achievement (and Other) Changes Under Chicago School
Reform,” Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 21:67-83.
Hoxby, Caroline, 2000, Peer Effects in the Classroom: Learning from Gender and Race
Variation,” NBER working paper 7867.
Jacob, Brian and Lars Lefgren, 2002, “The Impact of Teacher Training on Student Achievement:
Quasi-Experimental Evidence from School Reform Efforts in Chicago,” NBER Working
Paper No. 8916.
Jacob, Brian and Steven Levitt, 2001, “"Rotten Apples: An Investigation of the Prevalence and
Predictors of Teacher Cheating," Quarterly Journal of Economics, forthcoming.
Jepsen, Christopher and Steven Rivkin, 2001, “What is the Tradeoff Between Smaller Classes
and Teacher Quality,” Working paper, Public Policy Institute of California.
Kane, Thomas and Douglas Staiger, 2002, “The Promises and Pitfalls of Using Imprecise School
Accountability Measures,” Journal of Economic Perspectives 16:91-114.
Kozol, Jonathan, 1991, Savage Inequalities. New York: Crown Publishers.
Manski, Charles, 1993, “Identification of Endogenous Social Effects: The Reflection Problem,”
Review of Economic Studies 40:531-542.
Moulton, Brent, 1986, “Random Group Effects and the Precision of Regression Estimates,”
Journal of Econometrics 32:385-397.
Rivers, June and William Sanders, 2002, “Teacher Quality and Equity in Educational
Opportunity: Findings and Policy Implications,” in Izumi and Evers (eds.), Teacher
Quality, Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press.

32

Rivkin, Steven, Eric Hanushek, and John Kain, 2002, “Teachers, schools, and academic
achievement,” Working paper, University of Texas at Dallas.
Roderick, Melissa, 2001, “Educational Trends and Issues in the Region, the State and the
Nation,” In L. B. Joseph (ed.), Education Policy for the 21st Century: Challenges and
Opportunities in Standards-Based Reform, Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Sacerdote, Bruce, 2001, “Peer Effects with Random Assignment: Results for Dartmouth
Roommates,” Quarterly Journal of Economics 116:681-704.
Summers, Anita and Barbara Wolfe, 1977, “Do schools make a difference?” American Economic
Review 67:639-652.
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, The Condition of
Education 2000, NCES 2000-062, Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office,
2000.

33

Data Appendix

The student administrative records assign an eight-character identification to teachers.
The first three characters are derived from the teacher’s name (often the first 3 characters of the
last name) and the latter five reflect the teacher’s “position number” which is not necessarily
unique. In the administrative student data, several teacher codes arise implausibly few times.
When we can reasonably determine that the teacher code contains simple typographical errors,
we recode it in the student data. Typically, we will observe identical teacher codes for all but a
few students in the same classroom, during the same period, in the same semester, taking the
same subject, and a course level other than special education. These cases we assume are
typographical errors. Indeed, often the errors are quite obvious, as in the reversal of two numbers
is the position code.
A second problem we face in the teacher data occurs because a teacher’s position and
school number may change over time. We assume that administrative teacher records with the
same first and last name as well as the same birth date are the same teacher and adjust
accordingly. Finally, we face the problem of matching the teacher codes in the student-level
administrative records to the administrative data on teachers. We first match students to teachers
using the school number, a three-letter name code, and the position number for the combinations
that are unique in the teacher data.38 Next, we match students to teachers using the school
number, the first letter of the last name, and the position number, again for unique combinations.
Next, we match students and teachers using unique school number and position number
combinations, and finally we match any remaining students and teachers using unique
combinations of school number and the first three letters of the last name.

38

Note, some three-letter teacher codes were assigned manually for cases in which the teacher code did not
correspond to the first 3 letters of the teacher’s last name.

34

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for the Student Data

Sample Size
Total
1997
1998
1999

Test scores (grade equivalents)
Math, 9th grade
Math, 8th grade
Math change, 8th to 9th grade
Reading comprehension, 9th grade
Reading comprehension, 8th grade
Reading change, 8th to 9th grade
Demographics
Age
Female
Asian
African-American
Hispanic
Native American
Eligible for free school lunch
Eligible for reduced-price school lunch
Legal Guardian:
Dad
Mom
Nonrelative
Other relative
Stepparent
Schooling
Take algebra
Take geometry
Take computer science
Take calculus
Fraction honors math classes
Fraction regular math classes
Fraction essential math classes
Fraction basic math classes
Fraction special ed. math classes
Fraction nonlevel math classes
Fraction level missing math classes

All Students

Students with 8th
and 9th grade math
scores

Students with 8th
and 9th grade test
scores 1 year apart

84,190
29,302
27,340
27,548

64,457
21,992
20,905
21,560

52,991
17,941
16,936
18,114

Mean

Std Dev

9.07
7.75
1.15
8.50
7.64
0.66

2.74
1.55
1.89
2.94
1.94
2.02

9.05
7.90
1.15
8.50
7.82
0.67

2.71
1.50
1.89
2.89
1.88
2.02

9.21
8.07
1.14
8.63
8.01
0.62

2.64
1.41
1.75
2.88
1.80
1.95

14.8
0.497
0.035
0.549
0.311
0.002
0.703
0.091

0.8
0.500
0.184
0.498
0.463
0.047
0.457
0.288

14.7
0.511
0.033
0.571
0.304
0.002
0.721
0.097

0.8
0.500
0.179
0.495
0.460
0.046
0.448
0.295

14.6
0.522
0.036
0.562
0.307
0.002
0.728
0.103

0.7
0.500
0.185
0.496
0.461
0.046
0.445
0.303

0.241
0.620
0.041
0.038
0.002

0.428
0.485
0.197
0.191
0.050

0.244
0.627
0.039
0.034
0.002

0.429
0.484
0.195
0.182
0.047

0.253
0.619
0.037
0.032
0.002

0.435
0.486
0.189
0.177
0.046

0.826
0.099
0.003
0.0001
0.082
0.824
0.032
0.001
0.014
0.006
0.041

0.379
0.299
0.054
0.011
0.269
0.359
0.173
0.036
0.115
0.057
0.163

0.866
0.092
0.003
0.0001
0.094
0.827
0.029
0.001
0.009
0.006
0.035

0.341
0.288
0.057
0.010
0.286
0.355
0.163
0.031
0.093
0.055
0.142

0.952
0.022
0.003
0.0001
0.102
0.821
0.032
0.001
0.009
0.006
0.029

0.213
0.146
0.057
0.008
0.297
0.360
0.172
0.034
0.092
0.058
0.119

35

Mean

Std Dev

Mean

Std Dev

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for the Student Data

All Students
Fraction of math grades that are A
Fraction of math grades that are B
Fraction of math grades that are C
Fraction of math grades that are D
Fraction of math grades that are F
Fraction of math grades missing
Number of math/CS classes taken in 9th
grade
Number of times in 9th grade
Changed school within the year
Average class size among 9th grade math
classes
Cumulative GPA, Spring
Average absences in 9th grade math
Identified as disabled

Students with 8th
and 9th grade math
scores

Students with 8th
and 9th grade test
scores 1 year apart

0.084
0.131
0.202
0.234
0.309
0.041

0.254
0.293
0.346
0.366
0.426
0.163

0.085
0.140
0.220
0.250
0.270
0.035

0.254
0.299
0.353
0.372
0.406
0.142

0.094
0.153
0.234
0.252
0.238
0.029

0.264
0.308
0.356
0.366
0.382
0.119

2.1
1.17
0.034

0.4
0.41
0.180

2.1
1.13
0.030

0.4
0.36
0.170

2.1
1.00
0.027

0.4
0.00
0.163

22.7
1.71
13.7
0.021

7.5
1.08
16.6
0.143

23.2
1.82
11.4
0.024

7.4
1.04
13.5
0.154

23.6
1.93
9.7
0.022

7.5
1.03
11.4
0.147

Notes: The share of students disabled does not include students identified as learning disabled. Roughly 9
percent of CPS students in our estimation sample are identified as learning disabled.

36

Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for Math Test Score Data
Over Time and for Various Subgroups

1993-2000 Sample
1997-99 Sample
Yeara
1993
1994
1995
1996
1997
1998
1999
2000
Sex of Student ('97-'99)
Male
Female
Incomeb ('97-'99)
Low
High
Race/Ethnicity ('97-'99)
White
African-American
Asian
Native American
Hispanic

N
180,636
69,639

TAP
Mean
8.77
9.07

Std. Dev.
2.68
2.74

N
204,177
75,089

ITBS
Mean
7.65
7.75

Std. Dev.
1.53
1.55

21,893
21,761
21,884
22,600
23,850
22,570
23,219
22,859

8.52
8.07
8.49
8.08
8.79
8.89
9.53
9.72

2.48
2.45
2.50
2.51
2.70
2.58
2.85
2.80

24,889
25,388
26,120
27,093
25,866
24,329
24,894
25,598

7.45
7.55
7.49
7.44
7.55
7.85
7.87
8.01

1.53
1.44
1.40
1.45
1.49
1.54
1.60
1.68

34,083
35,556

9.11
9.04

2.83
2.64

37,661
37,428

7.68
7.83

1.63
1.47

56,240
13,399

8.84
10.06

2.54
3.25

60,067
15,022

7.68
8.03

1.50
1.71

6,923
38,852
2,477
150
21,237

10.93
8.53
12.04
10.47
9.11

3.21
2.40
3.29
3.20
2.57

6,578
43,484
2,272
165
22,590

8.57
7.51
9.25
8.27
7.82

1.67
1.49
1.62
1.70
1.45

Notes: Authors' calculations from the Chicago Public School District administrative student data for
students enrolled in 9th grade from the 1992-93 through 1999-2000 academic years. Test scores are
reported in terms of grade equivalents. Average TAP scores refer to the math portion of the Test of
Achievement and Proficiency administered in the 9th grade. Average ITBS scores refer to 9th graders' 8th
grade test scores on the math portion of the Iowa Test of Basic Skills.
a
Year refers to the Spring of the students' 9th grade school year.
b
Low income is defined as receiving free or reduced price school lunch.

37

Table 3
Mean Variance by Teacher of Lagged Student Test Score Measures

Observed
Perfect sorting across teachers w/in school
Randomly assigned teachers w/in school
Perfect sorting across teachers
Randomly assigned teachers

8th grade
scores
0.982
0.346
1.077
0.023
1.175

Observed
Perfect sorting across teachers w/in school
Randomly assigned teachers w/in school
Perfect sorting across teachers
Randomly assigned teachers

1.010
0.373
1.102
0.025
1.197

Observed
Perfect sorting across teachers w/in school
Randomly assigned teachers w/in school
Perfect sorting across teachers
Randomly assigned teachers

0.982
0.346
1.077
0.026
1.223

Fall 1997
6th to 7th
change
0.745
0.228
0.771
0.012
0.768
Fall 1998
0.750
0.248
0.770
0.012
0.769
Fall 1999
0.745
0.228
0.771
0.013
0.778

7th to 8th
change
0.781
0.248
0.794
0.011
0.790

0.807
0.273
0.823
0.016
0.820
0.781
0.248
0.794
0.017
0.848

Notes: In each cell, we report the average variance by teacher for the lagged math test
measure reported at the top of the column when students are assigned to teachers based
on the row description. Observed calculates the average variance for the observed
assignment of students to teachers. Perfect sorting assigns students to teachers either
within school or across schools based on the test score measure at the top of the column.
Randomly assigned teachers sorts students into teachers either within or across schools
based on a randomly generated number from a uniform distribution. The random
assignments are repeated 100 times before averaging across all teachers and all random
assignments. The top panel reports averages for the Fall of 1997, the middle panel for 1998
and the bottom panel for 1999. Calculations for the spring semesters are very similar.

38

Table 4
Descriptive Statistics for the Math Teachers Matched to
Teachers in the Student Data
Mean

Std. Dev.

Demographics
Age
Female
African-American
White
Hispanic
Asian
Native American

45.0
0.529
0.372
0.467
0.091
0.060
0.009

10.5
0.500
0.484
0.499
0.289
0.239
0.096

Human capital
BA major education
BA major all else
BA major math
BA major science
BA university, US News 1
BA university, US News 2
BA university, US News 3
BA university, US News 4
BA university, US News 5
BA university, US News else
BA university missing
BA university local
Master's degree
Ph.D.
Certificate, bilingual education
Certificate, child
Certificate, elementary
Certificate, high school
Certificate, special education
Certificate, substitute
Potential experience
Tenure at CPS
Tenure in position
Percent time in position

0.186
0.262
0.474
0.078
0.088
0.078
0.153
0.078
0.016
0.566
0.022
0.597
0.511
0.014
0.117
0.019
0.098
0.839
0.109
0.380
20.4
13.3
5.6
1.0

0.389
0.440
0.500
0.268
0.284
0.268
0.361
0.268
0.124
0.496
0.146
0.491
0.500
0.119
0.322
0.137
0.298
0.368
0.312
0.486
10.3
10.0
6.0
0.0

Number of Observations

645

Notes: There are 856 teachers identified from the student
estimation sample that have at least 15 student-semesters for
math classes over the 1997-1999 sample period. The
descriptive statistics above apply to the subset of these
teachers that can be matched to the teacher administrative
records from Chicago Public Schools.

39

Table 5
Distribution of the Estimated Teacher Effects

Distribution of teacher fixed effects:
10th percentile
25th percentile
50th percentile
75th percentile
90th percentile

Unweighted
-0.42
-0.26
-0.07
0.21
0.67

Weighted
-0.36
-0.23
-0.07
0.16
0.57

90-10 gap
75-25 gap
Variance
Adjusted Variance

1.09
0.47
0.208
0.172

0.94
0.39
0.155

Adjusted R2

0.68

p-value for the F-test on:
teacher fixed effects

0.000

8th grade math score and year dummies

Math scores units
Number of students
Number of teachers
# of students threshold

0.000

Grade Equivalents
52,991
857
15

Notes: All results are based on a regression of 9th grade math test score on 8th grade
math test score, teacher semester counts, and year indicators.

40

Table 6
Quartile Rankings of Estimated Teacher Effects in Years t and t+1:
Percent of Teachers by Row

Quartile in year t+1
1

2

3

4

1

40

28

24

8

2

25

33

33

9

3

24

31

26

19

4

5

11

24

60

Quartile in
year t

χ 2 test of random quartile assignment: p < 0.001
Notes: Quartile rankings are based on teacher effects estimated for each year based on the
specification in column 1 of table 5.

41

Table 7
Distribution of the Estimated Teacher Effects
(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

Variance
Adjusted Variance
Weighted Variance

0.208
0.172
0.155

0.148
0.115
0.110

0.092
0.059
0.061

0.096
0.054
0.061

0.080
0.040
0.047

p-value, F-test of teacher effects

0.000

0.000

0.000

0.000

0.000

p-value, F-test of lagged test score and year

0.000
0.000

0.000

p,value F-test for basic student covariates
p-value, F-test for school effects
p-value, F-test for additional student, peer,
and neighborhood covariates
Included Covariates
Year fixed effects
Basic student covariates
Additional student covariates
Math peer covariates
Neighborhood covariates
School fixed effects
# of students threshold

0.000

0.000
yes
no
no
no
no
no
15

yes
yes
no
no
no
no
15

yes
yes
yes
yes
yes
no
15

0.000
yes
yes
no
no
no
yes
15

yes
yes
yes
yes
yes
yes
15

Notes: All results are based on a regression of 9th grade math test score on 8th grade math test score, teacher
semester counts, year indicators, and other covariates as listed in the table. All test scores are measured in grade
equivalents. Student covariates include gender, race, age, guardianship, number of times in 9th grade, free or reducedprice lunch status, whether changed school during school year, and average math class size. Additional student
covariates include level and subject of math classes, cumulative GPA, class rank, disability status, and whether school
is outside of the student's residential neighborhood. Peer covariates include the 10th, 50th, and 90th percentile of math
class absences and 8th grade math test scores in 9th grade math classes. Neighborhood covariates include median
family income, median house value, and fraction of adult population that fall into five education categories. All
neighborhood measures are based on 1990 census tract data. There are 52,991 students and 857 teachers in each
specification.

42

Table 8
Quartile Rankings of Estimated Teacher Effects in Years t and t+1:
Percent of Teachers by Row

Quartile in year t+1
1

2

3

4

1

34

34

13

19

2

32

25

31

12

3

14

25

38

24

4

16

21

23

40

Quartile in
year t

χ 2 test of random quartile assignment: p < 0.000
Notes: Quartile rankings are based on teacher effects estimated for each year based on the
specification including lagged math test score, year indicators, and all student, peer, and
neighborhood covariates.

43

Table 9
Distribution of the Estimated Teacher Effects

100

Test Scores
Measured in
Percentiles

Trimming top
and bottom 3
percent in
changes

Low

Middle

High

9.08

Student Threshold
50

Ability Level

Dependent variable mean

9.21

9.21

37.88

7.07

8.71

11.81

Mean test score gain

1.14

1.14

-2.08

1.06

0.54

0.67

2.22

Number of teachers
Number of students

542
52,991

335
52,991

857
52,991

849
50,426

564
16,892

513
18,625

418
17,474

Without school effects
Variance of teacher effects
Adjusted variance
Weighted variance
p-value, F-test for teacher effects

0.050
0.038
0.047
0.000

0.049
0.042
0.045
0.000

7.30
4.25
4.90
0.000

0.071
0.042
0.045
0.000

0.050
0.020
0.037
0.000

0.095
0.057
0.069
0.000

0.085
0.046
0.059
0.000

With school effects
Variance of teacher effects
Adjusted variance
Weighted variance
p-value, F-test for teacher effects

0.034
0.017
0.031
0.000

0.030
0.021
0.028
0.000

7.07
3.24
4.30
0.000

0.064
0.027
0.036
0.000

0.056
0.016
0.043
0.000

0.106
0.050
0.078
0.000

0.078
0.018
0.053
0.000

Notes: See notes to table 7. Ability level is assigned in thirds based on the 8th grade test score distribution. High ability students have scores above 8.7,
middle ability students score between 7.5 and 8.7, and low ability students have scores of less than 7.5. All regressions include the student, peer, and
neighborhood covariates included in the Table 7, column 3 and 5 specifications.

44

Table 10
Distribution of the Estimated Teacher Effects

Math Only
Math Teachers
Variance
Adjusted Variance
Weighted Variance
Number of math teachers

0.091
0.059
0.061
857

English Teachers
Variance
Adjusted Variance
Weighted Variance
Number of English teachers
F-statistic for math teacher effects
F-statistic for English teacher effects

4.6

Teacher Quality Estimates
Math and English
English Only
0.092
0.042
0.053
857

0.067
0.012
0.042
1044

0.067
0.027
0.051
1044

2.0
1.7

3.8

Notes: See notes to Table 7. There are 52,991 students in each specification. Column (1)
is the same as column (3) of Table 7. Column (2) additionally includes controls for the
English teachers, while column (3) only controls for English teachers

45

Table 11
Impact of Observable Characteristics on Teacher Fixed Effects
(1)

(2)
0.045 *
(0.023)
0.048
(0.052)
0.024
(0.026)
-0.069
(0.046)
0.024
(0.115)
0.012
(0.010)
-0.001
(0.001)
0.011
(0.009)

Female
Asian
Black
Hispanic
Native American
Potential experience
squared
cubed (divided by 1000)
Potential experience < 1
Potential experience = 1
Masters
PhD
BA major: education
BA major: math
BA major: science

0.001
(0.022)
-0.077
(0.095)
0.096 *
(0.033)
0.047 *
(0.026)
0.064
(0.043)

-0.015
(0.023)
-0.064
(0.096)
0.076 *
(0.039)
0.061 *
(0.029)
0.076 *
(0.045)
-0.071
(0.044)
0.097
(0.091)
0.068
(0.046)
0.010
(0.038)
0.023
(0.043)
0.028
(0.029)
-0.009
(0.012)
0.000
(0.001)
0.000
(0.000)
0.018
(0.043)

bilingual certificate
child certificate
elementary certificate
high school certificate
special ed certificate
substitute certificate
Tenure at CPS
squared
cubed (divided by 1000)

-0.005
(0.007)
0.000
(0.001)
0.000
(0.000)

BA univ = level 1
46

(3)
0.041 *
(0.023)
0.043
(0.052)
0.019
(0.026)
-0.066
(0.046)
0.022
(0.114)

-0.031
(0.052)
0.278
(0.186)
-0.009
(0.023)
-0.057
(0.096)
0.079 *
(0.039)
0.061 *
(0.029)
0.083 *
(0.045)
-0.073
(0.044)
0.098
(0.091)
0.065
(0.045)
0.008
(0.038)
0.024
(0.043)
0.035
(0.029)
-0.003
(0.011)
0.000
(0.001)
0.000
(0.000)
0.017
(0.043)

Table 11
Impact of Observable Characteristics on Teacher Fixed Effects
(1)

(2)

BA univ = level 2
BA univ = level 3
BA univ = level 4
BA univ = level 5
BA univ local

adjusted R2
0.013
# of teachers with observables
645

(3)

0.049
(0.044)
0.014
(0.033)
0.025
(0.045)
0.098
(0.091)
-0.002
(0.026)

0.041
(0.044)
0.014
(0.033)
0.023
(0.045)
0.108
(0.090)
-0.004
(0.026)

0.044
645

0.044
645

Notes: * = significant at 10 percent level. The dependent variable is
teacher quality estimated using the table 7, column 3 specification. Each
specification also includes a constant. Potential experience is calculated
as age-education-6 and is the teacher's average over the 3 years.

47

Working Paper Series (continued)
State-Contingent Bank Regulation With Unobserved Action and
Unobserved Characteristics
David A. Marshall and Edward Simpson Prescott

WP-02-24

Local Market Consolidation and Bank Productive Efficiency
Douglas D. Evanoff and Evren Örs

WP-02-25

Life-Cycle Dynamics in Industrial Sectors. The Role of Banking Market Structure
Nicola Cetorelli

WP-02-26

Private School Location and Neighborhood Characteristics
Lisa Barrow

WP-02-27

Teachers and Student Achievement in the Chicago Public High Schools
Daniel Aaronson, Lisa Barrow and William Sander

WP-02-28

8