View original document

The full text on this page is automatically extracted from the file linked above and may contain errors and inconsistencies.

Search

MACROBLOG

December 5, 2016

Using Judgment in Forecasting: Does It Matter?

Many professional forecasters use statistical models when making their near-term projections for real gross domestic product (GDP)
growth. A 2013 special survey on the forecasting methods of the Survey of Professional Forecasters
found that 18 out of 21
respondents featured a statistical model prominently in their current-quarter economic projections. Nevertheless, there is fairly
compelling evidence that many professional forecasters incorporate judgment in their forecasts of the first estimate of real GDP
growth for a quarter—even when much of the source data used to construct the GDP estimate are available.
In the October 2016 Wall Street Journal Economic Forecasting Survey (WSJ), the most common panelist projection for annualized
third-quarter real GDP growth was 2.5 percent, and the second most common one was 3.0 percent. The first digit after the decimal
point, or tenths digit, of these two numbers are "5" and "0." Of the 58 individual forecasts of third-quarter growth in the survey, 21 had
a tenths digit of "0" or "5," a total that is almost twice as large as we would expect if all tenths digits were equally likely to be
submitted.
This pattern isn't unique to the most recent quarter's GDP forecast. The following chart shows the historical frequency of the tenths
digit in past WSJ surveys for first estimates of real GDP growth over the period from the first quarter of 2003 to the third quarter of
2016, made about three weeks before the release.

Almost 40 percent of these 2,390 forecasts have a tenths digit of "0" or "5." In contrast, the historical distribution of published first
estimates of real GDP growth from the fourth quarter of 1991 to the third quarter of 2016 and real gross national product (the most
common measure of U.S. production in an earlier era) growth from the third quarter of 1965 to the third quarter of 1991 has a tenths
digit of either "0" or "5" only 18 percent of the time. The historical Atlanta Fed's GDPNow forecasts have a "0" or a "5" tenths digit
only 15 percent of the time.
More formally, one easily can reject the hypothesis at the 1 percent significance level that the tenths digit of the WSJ panelist
forecasts are either uniformly distributed or follow the Benford distribution for tenths digits after rounding to the nearest tenth (see
this paper by economists Stefan Gunnel and Karl-Heinz Todter , who found similar relative frequencies of "0s" and "5s" in

A

A

A

professional forecasts of German GDP growth and consumer price index inflation).
If we assume that near-term GDP growth forecasts with a tenths digit of "0" or "5" typically involve more judgment than forecasts with
another tenths digit, a natural question is whether these more judgmental forecasts are less accurate than others. Of the 2,390 WSJ
growth forecasts mentioned above, the ones with a tenths digit of "0" or "5" (after rounding to the nearest tenth) had an average error
of 0.786 percentage points without regard to sign, and the others had an average error of 0.743 percentage points. These accuracy
metrics are not statistically different at even the 10 percent significance level. Moreover, because of the panel nature of WSJ
forecasts, we can measure how often a forecaster has a tenths digit of "0" or "5" (after rounding). Of the 44 panelists who submitted
at least 30 three-week-ahead GDP forecasts during the period of the first quarter of 2003 through the third quarter of 2016, the
correlation of the panelists "0" or "5" tenth digit frequency and their average error without regard to sign is only 0.13 and not
significantly different from 0.
Although at least some professional forecasters appear to make judgmental adjustments to their near-term GDP projections, the
evidence presented here does not suggest it comes, on average, at the cost of accuracy.
By Pat Higgins, an associate policy adviser and economist in the Atlanta Fed's research department

January 7, 2016 in Education, Employment, Labor Markets | Permalink