View original document

The full text on this page is automatically extracted from the file linked above and may contain errors and inconsistencies.

Federal Reserve Bank San Francisco | Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy? |

FEDERAL RESERVE BANK
OF SAN FRANCISCO
HOME

RESEARCH

BANKING SUPERVISION

EDUCATION

Search SF Fed
COMMUNITY

CASH

OUR DISTRICT

Home > Economic Research > Publications > Economic Letter > Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy?

Economic Research

Our Economists | Publications | About Us

FRBSF ECONOMIC LeTTer
2008-22

July 18, 2008

Subscribe

RSS Feed

Share

« More Economic Letters

Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy?
Elizabeth Cascio
Comparing academic performance across countries
Why are age profiles of skill different across countries?
Summary
References
Young Americans entering the labor market today face substantial competition. Employers can look all
over the world for workers with the skills to meet their firms’ needs. Are young Americans ready for
these challenges? The answer isn’t obvious. On the one hand, U.S. high school students consistently
perform worse on international standardized tests than students in other industrialized countries; on the
other hand, the United States generally has maintained the highest college completion rate in the world.
Sorting out the net effect of these two phenomena on young Americans’ readiness to compete in a
global job market has been difficult given the dearth of suitable data.
This Economic Letter summarizes new research by Cascio, Clark, and Gordon (2008) (hereafter CCG)
that uses data from the International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS), fielded in the 1990s, to address this
issue. The authors estimate the skill levels of 16- and 17-year-olds and 26- to 30-year-olds for the
United States and other high-income countries. Consistent with other assessments of the school-age
population, the IALS data show that U.S. 16- and 17-year-olds perform poorly relative to their
counterparts in other nations. By their late 20s, however, those in the U.S. group in the IALS data
compare much more favorably to their counterparts abroad, suggesting that they are able to “catch up”
in college or beyond. The authors then discuss why the U.S. “age profile of skill” is so different from that
in other countries.
Comparing academic performance across countries
Although cross-country data on educational attainment and wage rates are available, they are poor
proxies for skill, as they embody unmeasured differences in school quality, labor market regulation, and
other country-specific factors. Using data from the relatively recent IALS sidesteps these problems. The
IALS measured the information-processing skills of 16- to 65-year-olds in different countries in 1994,
1996, and 1998. Respondents were given several passages of text and, following each, were posed a

http://www.frbsf.org/economic-research/publications/economic-letter/2008/july/young-americans-global-economy-competition/[2/11/2015 3:39:12 PM]

Federal Reserve Bank San Francisco | Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy? |

series of questions to measure skills in three domains: quantitative (applying “arithmetic operations…to
numbers embedded in printed materials”), prose (understanding and using “information in texts”), and
document (locating and using “information in various formats”) (OECD 1995). The test was intended to
measure “literacy” broadly (“general skills), rather than occupation-specific knowledge or academic
proficiency. Scores on the IALS tests were categorized from level one (the ability to locate information in
text) to level five (the ability to locate information in dense text with multiple distractors and to make
high-level inferences). Performance on the IALS test is strongly correlated with individual earnings,
though the strength of that correlation and the responsiveness of earnings to changes in IALS scores
differ considerably across countries (Blau and Kahn 2005).
Based on the IALS, CCG estimate the share of the population
that is highly skilled—that is, who scored at least at level-four
proficiency on the test on average—for a set of developed
countries. Figure 1 displays separate shares for 16- and 17year-olds and for 26- to 30-year-olds. Countries are ordered
from highest to lowest based on the performance of teenagers
(solid black bars). To eliminate changes in the demographic
composition of the population associated with high immigration
rates of young adults, data are reported only for native-born
citizens of each country.
The results for 16- and 17-year-olds confirm the findings from
previous international standardized tests. On average, nativeborn U.S. teenagers performed worse on the IALS test than
teenagers in any other country: Only 4.7% achieved at least
level-four proficiency. In contrast, in the highest ranking
country, Sweden, more than 35% of students achieved at least
level-four proficiency. Across all nations other than the United States, on average 16% of 16- and 17year-olds were highly skilled.
The gray bars, representing 26- to 30-year-olds, portray a very different picture. While the gap between
the highest performing country, Sweden, and the United States narrowed only slightly, the gap between
the United States and the average of developed countries narrowed substantially: 23.0% of U.S. 26- to
30-year-olds were proficient at level four or five, and 24.7% of this group in the other countries
surveyed were similarly skilled. The U.S. position thereby moved from the bottom to the middle of the
group.
The striking change in the U.S. ranking from the teenage years to young adulthood highlights an
important difference in the “age profile of skill” across countries. While substantial learning takes place
after high school in nearly all countries, these gains are particularly large in the United States. In fact,
except for Norway, the age profile of skill is steeper in the United States than in any other country in the
sample.
Why are age profiles of skill different across countries?
Why do U.S. 26- to 30-year-olds perform so much better on the IALS test than U.S. 16- and 17-yearolds? Why are such gains not observed in other rich countries? CCG argue that the steep age profile of
skill observed in the U.S. may have emerged because the costs to the individual of investing in
generalskills in this country are lower and the benefits are higher than in many other countries. In
particular, on the cost side, access to university education in many developed nations other than the
U.S. is controlled by the government along two dimensions. First, students are “tracked” during the
equivalent of K-12; that is, some are on a track that makes them “university eligible,” and therefore
they take courses that prepare them for college, while some are on a vocational track where the courses

http://www.frbsf.org/economic-research/publications/economic-letter/2008/july/young-americans-global-economy-competition/[2/11/2015 3:39:12 PM]

Federal Reserve Bank San Francisco | Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy? |

they take would make competing for a place in a college difficult if not impossible. Second, the number
of available admissions to universities is limited and changes infrequently. Therefore, there is potentially
fierce competition for few places in university classrooms. In contrast, there is less educational tracking
in the U.S., so more students of varied educational backgrounds and ages can undertake the
appropriate preparation to apply to colleges and universities. Furthermore, there is a relatively high
level of private provision of college in the U.S., which allows for a greater ability to expand educational
opportunities to meet higher demand.
If the comparatively greater access to—that is, low cost of
entry into—the U.S. higher education system produces
greater acquisition of general skills, the United States should
have a relatively high college completion rate to match its
relatively steep age profile of skill. The data in Figure 2
suggest that this is true. The figure also shows that the
fraction of a country’s population receiving a bachelor’s
degree (or equivalent) between ages 16-17 and 26-30 is
strongly positively correlated with its age profile of skill. This
suggests that individuals’ investments in higher education
raise general skills in the average country, even when
university education tends to be more track-oriented and
focused on a particular field than it is in the U.S.
Differences in the benefits from acquiring general skills also
may be important in explaining differences across countries
in the age profile of skill. For instance, a relatively high
monetary return to college education may also have contributed to the relatively high U.S. college
completion rates observed in the IALS. Similarly, in other countries, there are incentives to invest in
vocational education over university education. For example, the German government heavily subsidizes
participation in apprenticeships, which combine on-the-job and classroom training for specific
professions. Thus, even though Germans and Americans spend about the same amount of time in school
after age 17, Germans are more likely than Americans to allocate that time toward vocational education.
While it has been argued that German apprenticeships bestow relatively general skills, it does not
appear that vocational education pays off in this way in the average country: Compared to its strong
correlation with college completion shown in Figure 2, the age profile of skill in a country is weakly
correlated with increases in years spent in school overall after age 17.
Summary
The research described in this Letter suggests that the skills of young Americans entering the labor
market may not be as low as internationally standardized tests historically suggest: While the average
U.S. teenager has a low probability of being able to perform high-level inferences, Americans in their
late 20s are as literate as their counterparts in a number of similar-income countries. These gains in skill
appear to be a by-product of the low-cost, high-return nature of investment in general skills in the
United States.
Will U.S. young adults continue to rank favorably in the distribution of skill across countries in the years
to come? Perhaps not. While there is considerably less private provision of higher education in countries
other than the United States, many did substantially expand public funding for universities in the 1980s
and 1990s, increasing accessibility accordingly. Thus, many countries that significantly lagged the
United States in college graduation only a decade ago—Finland, Sweden, and the United Kingdom,
among others—now have comparable if not higher graduation rates. While it is unclear whether these
countries will embrace the U.S. model of higher education, the educational battleground of the near
future seems likely to move beyond the elementary and secondary level.

http://www.frbsf.org/economic-research/publications/economic-letter/2008/july/young-americans-global-economy-competition/[2/11/2015 3:39:12 PM]

Federal Reserve Bank San Francisco | Can Young Americans Compete in a Global Economy? |

Elizabeth Cascio
Dartmouth College and
Visiting Scholar, FRBSF
References
Blau, Francine D., and Lawrence M. Kahn. 2005. “Do Cognitive Test Scores Explain Higher U.S. Wage
Inequality?” Review of Economics and Statistics 87(1) pp. 184-193.
Cascio, Elizabeth, Damon Clark, and Nora Gordon. 2008. “Education and the Age Profile of Literacy into
Adulthood.” Journal of Economic Perspectives, forthcoming.
OECD. 1995. Literacy, Economy, and Society: Results of the First International Adult Literacy Survey.
Paris and Ottawa: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, and Statistics Canada.
Subscribe

RSS Feed

Share

Opinions expressed in FRBSF Economic Letter
do not necessarily reflect the views of the
management of the Federal Reserve Bank of
San Francisco or of the Board of Governors of
the Federal Reserve System. This publication is
edited by Sam Zuckerman and Anita Todd.
Permission to reprint must be obtained in
writing.

Site Policies

More Economic Letters
Please send editorial comments and requests for reprint permission
to

Research Library
Attn: Research publications, MS 1140
Federal Reserve Bank of San Francisco
P.O. Box 7702
San Francisco, CA 94120

Privacy

Contact Us

Work for Us

Federal Reserve Bank of San Francisco © 2015

http://www.frbsf.org/economic-research/publications/economic-letter/2008/july/young-americans-global-economy-competition/[2/11/2015 3:39:12 PM]